![]() Talic, Irma ![]() Scientific Conference (2019, August 16) Detailed reference viewed: 73 (4 UL)![]() Hausen, Jennifer ![]() ![]() Scientific Conference (2019, August 13) The internal/external (I/E) frame of reference model postulates that achievement influences the formation of academic self-concept (ASC) via social and dimensional comparison processes. Its recent ... [more ▼] The internal/external (I/E) frame of reference model postulates that achievement influences the formation of academic self-concept (ASC) via social and dimensional comparison processes. Its recent extension as the generalized I/E model allows further domains than math and verbal achievement. However, intelligence facets as an indicator of achievement have been neglected within the GI/E model framework. Therefore, the purpose of this research is to explore the influences of three intelligence facets (verbal, numerical, figural) on domain-specific ASCs beyond grades and achievement scores. We drew on N=382 German students to analyze verbal, numerical, and figural intelligence, German and math achievement, self-reported grades and ASCs in four domains. We performed a structural equation model using Mplus with grades, achievement scores and intelligence facets specified as predictors and domain-specific ASCs as criteria. Positive paths were found from math, physics, German and English grade to their corresponding self-concepts. A positive path was found between physics grade and math ASC while negative paths were found from English grade to math and physics ASC and from math grade to German, physics and English ASC. The path coefficients from math achievement to math ASC as well as to physics ASC were positive; German achievement was negatively related to math ASC. Lastly, numerical intelligence was positively related to physics ASC, and verbal intelligence was positively related to English ASC. Thus, intelligence, specifically numerical and verbal intelligence, seem to be valid predictors of domain-specific ACSs beyond grades and achievement test scores. [less ▲] Detailed reference viewed: 110 (12 UL)![]() ; Krieger, Florian ![]() in PLoS ONE (2019), 14(8), Detailed reference viewed: 74 (5 UL)![]() ![]() Franzen, Patrick ![]() ![]() ![]() Scientific Conference (2019, August) Detailed reference viewed: 93 (16 UL)![]() ![]() Niepel, Christoph ![]() ![]() ![]() Scientific Conference (2019, February) Detailed reference viewed: 44 (0 UL)![]() ; ; Greiff, Samuel ![]() in Computers and Education (2019), 128 Complex problem solving (CPS) is a highly transversal competence needed in educational and vocational settings as well as everyday life. The assessment of CPS is often computer-based, and therefore ... [more ▼] Complex problem solving (CPS) is a highly transversal competence needed in educational and vocational settings as well as everyday life. The assessment of CPS is often computer-based, and therefore provides data regarding not only the outcome but also the process of CPS. However, research addressing this issue is scarce. In this article we investigated planning activities in the process of complex problem solving. We operationalized planning through three behavioral measures indicating the duration of the longest planning interval, the delay of the longest planning interval and the variance of intervals between each two successive interactions. We found a significant negative average effect for our delay indicator, indicating that early planning in CPS is more beneficial. However, we also found effects depending on task and interaction effects for all three indicators, suggesting that the effects of different planning behaviors on CPS are highly intertwined. [less ▲] Detailed reference viewed: 171 (13 UL)![]() Mustafic, Maida ![]() ![]() in Developmental Psychology (2019), 55 Detailed reference viewed: 128 (9 UL)![]() ; ; Greiff, Samuel ![]() in Psychological Assessment (2019), 31 Detailed reference viewed: 75 (2 UL)![]() ; ; Greiff, Samuel ![]() in Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie (2019), 33 Detailed reference viewed: 68 (0 UL)![]() Stadler, Matthias ![]() ![]() ![]() in Intelligence (2019), 72 Detailed reference viewed: 72 (3 UL)![]() Greiff, Samuel ![]() in European Journal of Psychological Assessment (2019), 35 Detailed reference viewed: 38 (1 UL)![]() Mainert, Jakob ![]() ![]() in Journal of Business and Psychology (2019), 34 Detailed reference viewed: 135 (12 UL)![]() ; Greiff, Samuel ![]() in Journal of Dynamic Decision Making (2019), 5 Detailed reference viewed: 25 (1 UL)![]() Krieger, Florian ![]() ![]() in Journal of Research in Personality (2019) The aim of this brief report was to replicate the meta-analytic findings concerning the relationship between Big-Five personality and political orientation reported in Sibley, Osborne, and Duckitt (2012 ... [more ▼] The aim of this brief report was to replicate the meta-analytic findings concerning the relationship between Big-Five personality and political orientation reported in Sibley, Osborne, and Duckitt (2012) in a sample of N = 29,015 participants from four panels involving representative German samples. We replicated the expected significant correlations for Openness to Experience (r = ?0.07; 95% CI [?0.10, ?0.05]) and Conscientiousness (r = 0.06, 95% CI [0.05, 0.08]), but the effect sizes were smaller than in Sibley et al. (2012). We also found significant correlations for Agreeableness (r = ?0.04; 95% CI [?0.05, ?0.03]) and Neuroticism (r = ?0.04; 95% CI [?0.06, ?0.02]), indicating small but significant relations of additional Big-Five dimensions on political orientation. [less ▲] Detailed reference viewed: 218 (9 UL)![]() Krieger, Florian ![]() ![]() in Intelligence (2019), 72 It is well documented that figural matrices tests are harder to solve when multiple rules need to be induced because multiple rules are traditionally associated with a greater demand for dynamically ... [more ▼] It is well documented that figural matrices tests are harder to solve when multiple rules need to be induced because multiple rules are traditionally associated with a greater demand for dynamically managed sub-goals (goal management), which requires more working memory capacity (WMC). The current research addresses the necessity to apply selective encoding as a requirement that goes beyond the ability to manage goals when solving figural matrices. In the first study (N = 38), we found that selective encoding demands are present in items with multiple rules in addition to goal management demands. Furthermore, eye movement data indicated that rule induction was hampered when selective encoding demands were present. The second study (N = 127) de-monstrated that individuals' ability to filter relevant features in working memory was positively related to figural matrices items with selective encoding demands. Moreover, there was no evidence that other sources of WMC are related to goal management in figural matrices. Hence, this study provides preliminary evidence that fil-tering of relevant information in working memory is critical for solving figural matrices with multiple rules and challenges the view that goal management is the only driver of the relationship between WMC and performance in solving figural matrices with multiple rules. [less ▲] Detailed reference viewed: 146 (15 UL)![]() Stadler, Matthias ![]() ![]() in Frontiers in Psychology (2019), 10 Detailed reference viewed: 34 (4 UL)![]() Greiff, Samuel ![]() ![]() ![]() in Psychological Test and Assessment Modeling (2019), 61 Detailed reference viewed: 73 (2 UL)![]() Stadler, Matthias ![]() ![]() ![]() in Journal of Intelligence (2019), 7 Detailed reference viewed: 70 (4 UL)![]() ; Greiff, Samuel ![]() in European Journal of Psychological Assessment (2019), 35 Detailed reference viewed: 124 (0 UL)![]() Keller, Ulrich ![]() ![]() in European Journal of Psychological Assessment (2019), 35 Need for Cognition (NFC) signifies “the tendency for an individual to engage in and enjoy thinking” (Cacioppo & Petty, 1982, p. 116). Up to now, no scale of sufficient psychometric quality existed to ... [more ▼] Need for Cognition (NFC) signifies “the tendency for an individual to engage in and enjoy thinking” (Cacioppo & Petty, 1982, p. 116). Up to now, no scale of sufficient psychometric quality existed to assess NFC in children. Using data from three independent, diverse cross-sectional samples from Germany, Luxembourg, and Finland, we examined the psychometric properties of a new NFC scale intended to fill in this gap. In all samples, across grades levels ranging from 1 to 9, confirmatory factor analysis confirmed the hypothesized nested factor structure based on Mussel’s (2013) Intellect model, with one general factor Think influencing all items and two specific factors Seek and Conquer each influencing a subset of items. At least partial scalar measurement invariance with regard to grade level and sex could be demonstrated. The scale exhibited good psychometric properties and showed convergent and discriminant validity with an established NFC scale and other non-cognitive traits such as academic self-concept and interests. It incrementally predicted mostly statistically significant but relatively small portions of academic achievement variance over and above academic self-concept and interest. Implications for research on the development of NFC and its role as an investment trait in intellectual development are discussed. [less ▲] Detailed reference viewed: 249 (49 UL) |
||